Learner Centred? Or Learning Centred??

This article, adapted from work done on our Delta Module One course, focuses on the concepts of the learner centred and learning centred classroom, and asks:

a) why the early communicative approach placed so much emphasis on "learner-centredness" and the forms it took

b) why recently people have suggested that we need to shift to a "learning centred" approach and what form this might take


Section a

The early Communicative Approach (CA) developed due to advances in learning theories and linguistic theories which undermined the tenets of Audiolingualism (AL),  a highly T. centred method where materials and techniques (eg repetition, substitution and transformation drills) controlled all the language the Ls learnt and used. This was presented in a strict structural progression from “simple” to more complex with the aim of preventing error – repetition and reinforcement of the correct forms being thought to lead to the “habit formation” which was the basis of behaviourism – the learning theory on which AL was based. 

Behaviourism was first attacked by Chomsky, who pointed out that errors such as *“I goed there yesterday” (overgeneralisation) showed evidence that the Ls were, in fact, applying rules and that language learning must therefore have a cognitive base. This was reinforced by a constructivist view of learning (Piaget and others) which suggested that learning occurred not just through input, but that Ls would actively build on their previous knowledge to incorporate new information (as in the case of the child who knew the words “dire” and “ear” and therefore, on hearing the word “diarrhoea” understood it to mean “earache”). This meant that learner output should no longer be strictly controlled but that Ls had to be given space to “try things out”, even if this resulted initially in error. Learning was seen to occur through “doing”. In language learning terms this meant introducing free practice and fluency practice activities.

Constructivism was then extended to “social constructivism” (Vygotsky) which emphasised the collaborative nature of much learning. This contributed to the early CA changing the typical c/r interaction pattern from a predominantly T-led, T/class format to one where PW and GW would predominate. Working in pairs/groups, the learners not only had more time to practise using the language than in T/class format, but could also learn from each other.

At the same time developments in educational psychology (the Humanistic Movement involving such writers as Rogers, Maslow and, in EFL, Stevick) were emphasising the need to take the Ls emotions, wants, and need for self-fulfilment into consideration. In ELT this led to an emphasis on the importance of T/L  rapport. The T. should no longer be an authority figure or “judge” of performance,  but as a guide who understood and accepted the Ls’ problems and appreciated them as people. This respect for personal qualities was also important in L/L rapport,  which would be improved by the collaborative PW/GW mentioned above, but also by the use of personalisation activities which allowed Ls to express themselves and get to know each other and the T. as people who had interesting life experiences, valid opinions etc.

Autonomy : Another tenet of humanistic approaches was that Ls would learn more effectively if they were given more control over their learning and encouraged to develop the skills to become autonomous learners, independent of the teacher. In EFL this movement was associated with the work of Holec and others at C.R.A.P.E.L in France.   Autonomy could involve giving Ls control of any or all aspects of the lesson, and led to the use of techniques such as syllabus negotiation, the teaching of skills which would enable the learner to continue learning independently outside the classroom (the development of inference skills or the ability to use dictionaries effectively) and peer and/or self-evaluation.

Social developments also contributed to the change in emphasis. Up to the 1960s,  overseas travel was minimal and, obviously, the internet did not exist. Most language learners were therefore school age, and learning a language as part of their intellectual development – not for immediate use. The AL approach, where it would take up to six years of study before the learner could start to communicate was therefore feasible. In the 1970s, however, international contact started to become more common. Foreign holidays were no longer limited to the rich, and gradually a knowledge of language became necessary in professional contexts. For the first time learners needed to use the language from the earliest stages of language learning. This focus on learner needs (a major element of the increase in the learner-centredness of courses at the time) was first evident in the notional/functional syllabuses which came out of the early work of the Council of Europe and emphasised the use of items as well as their structural form. This led to units on functional topics such as making polite requests, and making suggestions, which it was predicted that learners would need in communicative situations. Techniques such as information gap activities, roleplays etc were used to give the learners practice in using the language under realistic communicative conditions. And ultimately this led to the idea of tailor-made courses based on an analysis of the communicative needs of specific groups (eg English for nurses) or even tailor-made for an individual in a 121 situation. 

 

Section b

More recently people (eg Underhill and Scrivener – Demand High ELT) have started to ask if we haven’t gone too far in the other direction, leading to a situation where Ts believe their role should never be be “knower” or “informer” but always  “facilitator” of learning. However, they often do not understand what this term actually means or how they can facilitate learning at each stage of the lesson, and tend instead to do nothing. Examples of this are what has been termed “cocktail party” monitoring (the T. wandering aimlessly around the groups, listening but never intervening to correct, upgrade or explain problems), and what Demand High ELT (DHELT) has called “rubber-stamping” of answers – eg eliciting answers to activities until a correct reply is received and confirming it with an “excellent”, but never intervening to help the L who got it wrong for fear of embarrassing them. Trainee teachers have often been told never to say “no” to a L’s answer.

DHELT has suggested a number of remedies for the situation including giving the T. permission to “teach” – by for instance devoting ample time to the follow-up stage of a discussion to deal with errors or upgrade correct sentences which the Ls are capable of expressing in a more complex manner. For example,  if an intermediate L says “I hate Western films”, the T. can board this with a “smiley” to indicate it is correct, but then elicit other ways of saying “hate” (can’t stand, can’t bear, loathe etc). Various other film genres can then be elicited and boarded (thrillers, love stories, horror films etc) and the Ls can then each be asked to specify a genre they dislike, to practise the “dislikes” exponents and the lexical field together. However,  for this to happen and learning affordances to be fully exploited , much more time than is usually allowed has to be dedicated to the follow-up stage of  any discussion. Thornbury lists a number of ways that such upgrading and practice can take place in his article “F is for Focus on Form”. 

Another change that needs to be made in order to fully exploit learning affordances, and to present the “rubber-stamping” of answers is the reduction in the length of texts used for comprehension work, to bring them down to a length where what Thornbury terms “zero uncertainty” can be achieved. Modern coursebooks tend to use long texts slightly above the Ls’ level, on the basis that this input will lead to acquisition. We need to ask ourselves if it doesn’t rather lead to confusion. The T. doesn’t have time to focus on every item in the text or even to fully explain wrong answers to the comprehensions questions, and the Ls are often left with unanswered queries. Short texts avoid this problem. “Rubber-stamping” can be avoided by not confirming answers but boarding them all and then asking focus questions to point Ls to their mistakes – or with listening texts by repeating the target phrase, possibly more slowly each time getting ls to “vote” for the answer they now think is correct until all the Ls have heard what is said and understand the correct answer. The T can then focus on whatever caused the problem – for example the expected vs actual pronunciation of a phrase affected by features of connected speech. If this is done for all the features of the text predicted to be problematic, and if the T. finishes by asking whether Ls have any other questions about items in the text, I would argue that the T’s “interventionist” approach, even if it results in an increase in T/class work and teacher talking time, if more conducive to learning than simply ignoring the Ls’ problems and spending the time in additional pairwork.